NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.
National Research Council (US) and Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Developing a Strategy to Reduce and Prevent Underage Drinking; Bonnie RJ, O'Connell ME, editors. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 2004.
Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility.
Show detailsBonnie L. Halpern-Felsher and Michael Biehl
How do we prevent underage drinking in a society in which youth receive positive messages about alcohol coupled with warnings that alcohol is illegal and risky? Alcohol pervades our culture, and youth are well aware of its popularity. Adolescents and young adults witness the casual use of alcohol by parents, other adults, and peers, and alcohol is commonly displayed in advertisements, in movies, on television, on t-shirts, at sports events, and through similar venues. Therefore, it is not surprising that experimental or occasional use of alcohol is reported by the majority of U.S. adolescents (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2001), making it, by definition, a normative behavior during the second decade of life. This is not to say that underage alcohol consumption is condoned, as any alcohol use can lead to cognitive impairment, risk of injury or death if consumed before vehicle use, or violence. Futhermore, it is illegal for anyone under age 21.
In this chapter, we provide an overview of the developmental and environmental factors that have been implicated in normative underage alcohol use.1 In doing so, we apply a lifespan developmental systems approach (Baltes, 1987; Baltes, Reese, and Lipsitt, 1980; Lerner et al., 1994), whereby development is viewed as a lifespan process, with past events influencing current ones, which in turn influence future developments. A central tenet of lifespan models is that there is a dynamic, reciprocal interaction between the individual and his or her environment. Therefore, reducing risk behavior and promoting healthy pathways can only be accomplished through a fundamental understanding of the developmental and environmental factors that impinge on youths' lives.
In this chapter, we do not attempt to review all of the literature concerning the multitude of factors involved in adolescent alcohol use. Instead, we focus on developmental and environmental influences, with particular emphasis on factors we consider most amenable to prevention and intervention efforts. We refer readers to other, more extensive and lengthy reviews that have already made significant contributions to the field (e.g., Dielman, Butchart, Shope, and Miller, 1991; Halebsky, 1987; Hawkins, Catalano, and Miller, 1992; Vakalahi, 2001).
DEVELOPMENTAL INFLUENCES ON UNDERAGE ALCOHOL USE
Adolescence marks a period of great and rapid physical, cognitive, psychosocial, and emotional changes. All of these changes increase one's desire for more autonomy and decision making, which can result in risk taking, including alcohol use. Understanding how these individual-level developmental factors relate to alcohol use is critical in order to create developmentally appropriate and effective alcohol prevention and intervention programs. In this section, we review adolescent physical, cognitive, and psychosocial development, with particular emphasis on how such development influences underage alcohol consumption.
Physical Development
Adolescent development is typically marked by rapid and extreme biological changes—namely, the onset of puberty and menarche. During this time, hormones, brain development, and the environment interact to result in increased growth, change in voice characteristics, and the development of secondary sex characteristics.
Puberty often signals changes in one's own as well as others' expectations for the adolescent. For example, taller and more mature-looking adolescents are often expected to be more responsible and take on adult roles and behaviors earlier than less physically mature peers of the same age group. However, depending on the age at which puberty occurs, the adolescents' level of social and emotional maturation may not match their physical development. When this mismatch occurs, expectations for adult-like responsibilities may be met with negative or health-compromising self-perceptions and behaviors. For example, early maturing girls tend to show increased dissatisfaction with their bodies, have lower self-esteem, have school difficulties, and engage in problem behaviors such as drinking, smoking, and early sexual behavior as compared to late-maturing girls. In contrast, late-maturing boys express more dissatisfaction with their body image, tend to be less popular and less athletic, and perform less well academically than early maturing boys. The psychological and behavioral difficulties resulting from early or late pubertal onset can be ameliorated through parental involvement, communication, and support.
Cognitive and Psychosocial Development
During adolescence, thinking becomes more abstract and less concrete, and adolescents become more future oriented, allowing them to consider multiple aspects of any decision at one time, assess potential consequences of a decision, consider possible outcomes associated with various choices, and plan for the future. These cognitive changes are coupled with psychosocial development, including social perspective taking, susceptibility to peer pressure, and increased need for autonomy (e.g., Steinberg and Cauffman, 1996). Social perspective taking refers to the ability to recognize how the thoughts and actions of one person can influence those of another. Social perspective taking has been shown to gradually increase until about age 16 (Steinberg and Cauffman, 1996). Applied to underage drinking, it is expected that individuals who are more capable of social perspective taking will be more able to understand why underage alcohol use is not condoned and understand that not all people have the same views concerning alcohol use. Although generally an indicator of greater maturity, a downside of this new ability to understand different perspectives is that adolescents become highly concerned with peer conformity, which may make them more susceptible to peer pressure, including pressure to drink alcohol. A more detailed discussion of peer influences and social norms is presented later in this chapter.
These cognitive and psychosocial changes are also accompanied by adolescents' questioning of parental control and rules and their desire to be more autonomous, which often translates into their desire to participate in, and eventually dictate, their own decision making. The desire to be autonomous and make one's own decisions is considered an important hallmark of adolescence. The literature clearly indicates that the need to be more autonomous and be granted more decision making opportunities increases with age. Several other simultaneous changes during adolescence serve to increase adolescents' desire for autonomy. First, as indicated earlier, physical changes of puberty result in the adolescent seeing himself or herself as more deserving of privileges, and others tend to have similar expectations for youth. Second, increased time spent with peers leads to more experiences and comparisons of others' authority, power, and privileges. Finally, cultural and societal beliefs indicate that adolescence is a time to practice adult roles (Hill, 1973; see also Silverberg and Gondoli, 1996, for a review).
Perceptions of Alcohol-Related Risks
Two critical and overlapping areas of theory and research have emerged from our understanding of adolescent cognitive and psychosocial development: 1) adolescents' perceptions of risk, and 2) adolescent decision making. These areas have particular relevance to clinicians, policy makers, and researchers interested in understanding underage alcohol use. In this chapter, we discuss the notion of adolescent egocentrism, perceptions of invulnerability, and alcohol-related expectancies. For a discussion regarding alcohol-related decision making, see Jacobs, Chapter 5.
Both lay and scientific circles often have hypothesized that adolescents are not concerned about their health, that they make poor judgments about risks, and that they believe they are invulnerable to harm, and because of these perceptions, they engage in risk behaviors (see, for example, Social Cognitive Theory [Bandura, 1994], the Health Belief Model [Rosenstock, 1974], the Theory of Reasoned Action [Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975], the Theory of Planned Behavior [Ajzen, 1985], Self-Regulation Theory [Kanfer, 1970], and Subjective Culture and Interpersonal Relations Theory [Triandis, 1977]). The notion that adolescents do not adequately perceive risks largely stems from Elkind (1967, 1978), who argued that egocentric young adolescents hold an exaggerated sense of uniqueness and believe in a “personal fable”—that they are special and in some ways immune to the natural laws that pertain to others. Thus, they view themselves as invulnerable to harm, and therefore are more likely to engage in risk behaviors. Adolescent egocentrism and the associated perceptions of invulnerability are thought to become less evident as the adolescent matures cognitively.
Although Elkind's theory is appealing, it has not held up to empirical scrutiny. Evidence shows that adolescent egocentrism increases from childhood to early adolescence, and then gradually decreases through middle and late adolescence (De Rosenroll, 1987; Urberg and Robbins, 1984). But adolescent egocentrism does not appear to be linked to young adolescents' transition into formal operations (Gray and Hudson, 1984; Jahnke and Blanchard-Fields, 1993; Lapsley, Milstead, Quintana, Flannery, and Buss, 1986), nor has research supported a relationship between egocentrism and perceptions of invulnerability (Dolcini et al., 1989).
The majority of studies assessing the relationship between perceptions of invulnerability to harm and alcohol use have examined whether judgments of risk vary by behavioral engagement. Overall, results show that adolescents who have drunk alcohol perceive alcohol-related risks to be less likely to occur compared to adolescents without prior alcohol consumption (Goldberg, Halpern-Felsher, and Millstein, 2002; Halpern-Felsher et al., 2001; Hampson, Severson, Burns, Slovic, and Fisher, 2001; Kuther, 2002). Another set of studies has examined whether adolescents and adults differ in their estimates of risk. Contrary to hypotheses, most of this research has revealed that adults and adolescents give similar estimates of risk taking, including those related to alcohol use (e.g., Beyth-Marom, Austin, Fischhoff, Palmgren, and Jacobs-Quadrel, 1993; Quadrel, Fischhoff, and Davis, 1993).
Causal relationships between risk estimates and alcohol use must be made cautiously as most studies have used cross-sectional methodology, poor definitions of alcohol use, and inadequate assessments of risk perceptions (see Halpern-Felsher et al., 2001, for details). Despite lack of evidence, the concept of adolescent “invulnerability” remains pervasive in both scientific and lay circles, is used to explain adolescents' decisions to engage in potentially harmful behavior, and is incorporated into many intervention programs (Weinstein, 1983; Weisenberg, Kegeles, and Lund, 1980). Longitudinal, prospective studies are needed in order to fully understand the extent to which perceptions of low risk actually predict and motivate alcohol use.
Perceptions of Alcohol-Related Benefits
An emphasis on perceived risk alone may be inadequate to predict or change behavior because risk is only part of the behavioral decision making equation. What is missing is knowing the extent to which adolescents perceive benefits of risky behaviors. The decision making literature has argued that individuals should consider both the risks and benefits when making decisions (Baron, 1988; Weinstein and Fineberg, 1980). In addition, alcohol expectancy researchers have found that perceived benefits, in addition to perceived risks, are important predictors of drinking behavior (Christiansen, Goldman, and Inn, 1982; Christiansen, Roehling, Smith, and Goldman, 1989; Chen, Grube, and Madden, 1994; Goldberg et al., 2002; Grube, Chen, Madden, and Morgan, 1995; Jones, Corbin, and Fromme, 2001; Smith, Goldman, Greenbaum, and Christiansen, 1995; Wood, Nagoshi, and Dennis, 1992). More recently, Goldberg and colleagues (2002) found that, regardless of age, participants with more drinking experience perceived benefits to be more likely to occur, and risks less likely.
Indeed, adolescents' reasons for drinking alcohol often include an acknowledgment or identification of alcohol-related benefits, such as alcohol being used in social interactions to help them to reduce inhibitions, feel more relaxed (Jones et al., 2001; Wood et al., 1992), reduce tension, foster courage, and reduce worry (Prendergast, 1994).
Age, Gender, and Experiential Differences in Alcohol-Related Expectancies
Both positive and negative alcohol-related expectancies vary by age. By age 12, individuals with and without drinking experience have a well-formulated sense of alcohol-related expectancies (Christiansen et al., 1982; Jones et al., 2001). Although children's and adults' general alcohol expectancies are similar (Dunn and Goldman, 1996), negative expectancies are more often reported by younger children, with perceptions of alcohol-related benefits increasing with age (Goldberg et al., 2002; Miller, Smith, and Goldman, 1990). Specific expectancies also differ by age. Young adolescents (e.g., 12- to 14-year-olds) rank reduced tension and impaired behavioral functioning among the highest expectancies; 15- to 16-year-olds cite enhanced social and physical pleasure and modified social and emotional behavior; and older adolescents (e.g., 17- to 19-year-olds) list enhanced sexual performance and increased power among their highest expectancies (Christiansen et al., 1982).
There are also gender differences in alcohol expectations. Adolescent males typically perceive more positive and fewer negative consequences of alcohol use than do adolescent females. Furthermore, the frequency of alcohol use has been associated with global positive effects, sexual enhancement, and pleasure for men, but reduced tension for women (Jones et al., 2001). However, there were no differences between males and females in the relationship between amount of alcohol use and perceived outcomes.
Independent of age effects, prior experience with alcohol use also plays a large role in alcohol-related expectancies, alcohol-related decision making, and subsequent alcohol use (Christiansen et al., 1989; Christiansen et al., 1982; Chen et al., 1994; Goldberg et al., 2002; Grube et al., 1995; Jones et al., 2001; Smith et al., 1995; Wood et al., 1992). In particular, enhanced sexual feelings, power, and reduced tension have been reported by those with greater drinking experiences, while youth with little or no alcohol experiences have more global expectancies of increased pleasure (Christiansen et al., 1982).
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES ON UNDERAGE ALCOHOL USE
At the same time that youth are experiencing rapid physical, cognitive, psychosocial, and emotional changes, they are embedded within changing and multilayered contexts. These contexts and their reciprocal relationship with youth also must be understood in order to reduce underage drinking most effectively. In this section, we discuss the roles that parents, peers, and the larger society play in underage alcohol use.
Parents
Although multiple influences in adolescents' lives will determine their developmental course, parents have the single most important external influence on adolescents' development and behavior, including alcohol use. For more than 50 years, the relationship between parents' child-rearing orientations and child outcomes has been studied. This venture has resulted in a host of literature linking parenting styles to substance use (e.g., Lamborn, Mounts, Steinberg, and Dornbusch, 1991). Typically, researchers have found that children raised in homes in which parents were warm, loving, and involved, supported their children's independence and valued their children's opinions, and also set limits and monitored their children's activities were more likely to exhibit positive developmental outcomes and less likely to exhibit risk behaviors than were children reared in less supportive homes.
With respect to alcohol use, parental monitoring and involvement have emerged as particularly important parenting behaviors. Parental monitoring includes parental or guardian supervision and knowledge about their adolescents' activities and friends. Parental monitoring also entails setting clear expectations and having fair and direct consequences (Connell, Halpern-Felsher, Clifford, Crichlow, and Usinger, 1995; Connell and Halpern-Felsher, 1997; Halpern-Felsher, et al., 1997; Lee and Halpern-Felsher, 2001). Correlated with parental monitoring is parental involvement, which includes the parents having an active interest and participation in their child's activities and development. Parents can prevent or reduce adolescents' alcohol use in part by setting curfews, being aware of and participating in after-school and weekend activities, helping them to improve social skills, and reducing their adolescents' affiliation with peers who are engaging in risk activities (Beck and Lockhart, 1992; Cohen, Richardson, and LaBree, 1994; Steinberg, Fletcher, and Darling, 1994). Research on parental monitoring consistently shows protective effects on alcohol use (Barnes, Reifman, Farrell, and Dintcheff, 2000; Bogenschneider, Wu, Raffaelli, and Tsay, 1998; Reifman, Barnes, Dintcheff, Farrell, and Uhteg, 1998; DiClemente et al., 2001).
Parental norms and attitudes regarding adolescents' alcohol use (Ary, Tildesley, Hops, and Andrews, 1993; Baer, Barr, Bookstein, Sampson, and Streissguth, 1998; McGue, Sharma, and Benson, 1996; Stice, Barrera, and Chassin, 1998; Webb and Baer, 1995; Weinberg, Dielman, Mandell, and Shope, 1994) are also important influences on use (e.g., Sieving, Perry, and Williams, 2000). Studies have also shown that parents' own alcohol use is associated with underage drinking (e.g., Pandina and Johnson, 1989) as well as increased chance of experiencing alcohol-related negative consequences (Pandina and Johnson, 1990). Parents' use of alcohol is related to less family support, reduced parental monitoring, and less effective coping strategies, which in turn are related to a greater likelihood of alcohol use among adolescents (Barnes et al., 2000; e.g., Johnson and Pandina, 1993; Reifman et al., 1998).
Studies have also demonstrated that sibling alcohol use is a risk factor in a target individual's alcohol use (Ary et al., 1993; McGue et al., 1996). For example, McGue and colleagues (1996) showed that while parental alcohol use only had an effect for the biological children, sibling alcohol use was related to both adoptive and biological children's alcohol consumption. The effect was stronger if the sibling was similar in age, gender, and ethnicity.
Peers
Adolescents and adults spend a greater amount of time with their peers than with their family or other adults. Thus, peers become a major source of support and socialization for youth. Research shows that these peer networks can play a role in underage drinking. Adolescents are more likely to consume alcohol if they affiliate with peers who consume alcohol (Ary et al., 1993; Colder and Chassin, 1999; Fergusson, Horwood, and Lynskey, 1995; Curran, Stice, and Chassin, 1997; Dielman, Shope, Butchart, Campanelli, and Caspar, 1989; Sieving et al., 2000; Stice et al., 1998; Webster, Hunter, and Keats, 1994), have friends who offer them alcohol (Sieving et al., 2000), and are encouraged to use alcohol (Duncan, Duncan, and Hops, 1994; Keefe, 1994).
The potentially negative influence of peers on alcohol use can be ameliorated through positive adolescent-parent relationships, parental monitoring, and parental involvement (Parke and Ladd, 1992). For example, compared to less involved parents, parents who are more involved may oversee and monitor their children's peer relationships. Parents also have a significant amount of influence on their children's actual choice of friends, helping them to gravitate toward peers with fewer risk behaviors.
Several prevention efforts have attempted to alleviate the effects of peer influences on alcohol use. One area of programmatic focus has been to try to change perceptions of alcohol-related social norms. Such efforts have been based on research indicating that youth are aware of the normative nature of alcohol use and want to follow the crowd (Aas and Klepp, 1992; Barnes, Farrell, and Banerjee, 1995; Beck and Treiman, 1996; Olds and Thombs, 2001). Perceived use of alcohol by one's peers and friends independently predicts self-reported alcohol use (e.g., Olds and Thombs, 2001; Reifman et al., 1998), with peers having a greater influence on adolescent drinking than parents (Kuther, 2002). However, such programmatic efforts should proceed cautiously given evidence that two types of norms must be distinguished and acknowledged (Cialdini, Reno, and Kallgren, 1990; Kallgren, Reno, and Cialdini, 2000). One type, descriptive norms, are one's perceptions of what most others are doing. The second type, injunctive norms, are the perceptions of what other people think one should be doing or not be doing. Cialdini and colleagues argue that focusing on injunctive norms is more effective at changing behavior than targeting only descriptive norms.
Another effort to reduce the influence of peer pressure has been to teach and encourage drinking refusal self-efficacy (e.g., Bell, Ellickson, and Harrison, 1993; Ellickson, Bell, and Harrison, 1993). This concept, borrowed from Bandura's (19861997) general self-efficacy, refers to one's belief in his or her ability to resist urges or social pressures to drink, to drink in particular situations, or to consume large amounts of alcohol at one time. Adolescents with more positive self-efficacy are less likely to drink or drink excessively (Oei, Fergusson, and Lee, 1998; Webb and Baer, 1995), and those with fewer refusal skills are more likely to drink (Hays and Ellickson, 1996). Refusal skills may be a better predictor of problem drinking than alcohol expectancies, especially for heavy or frequent alcohol use (Connor, Young, Williams, and Ricciardelli, 2000; Oei et al., 1998).
Society and Community
We live in a society in which alcohol is ubiquitous, glamorized, and touted as a hallmark of adulthood. Advertisements for alcohol use are abundant, and alcohol is prominently displayed on television, in movies, in music, and at sports events. These societal messages compete with and even overshadow messages against underage drinking. Such mixed messages are particularly confusing to youth who are trying to make sense of their changing body and world, and who are trying to understand and meet societal expectations.
The extent to which alcohol consumption is considered to be the norm and is accepted within a given community contributes to variation in underage alcohol consumption. Underage drinking is less prevalent in communities in which youth are well monitored and where there is greater policing and enforcement of vendors who sell alcohol to youth. An especially vulnerable time in which youth particularly need supervision is after school (3 to 6 p.m.). Youth spend approximately 40 to 50 percent of their hours in discretionary time (Larson and Verma, 1999), with much of it unsupervised for older adolescents. Youth who participate in after-school programs, such as sports, clubs, library-based activities, and youth-serving organizations, are less likely to use alcohol (Eccles and Barber, 1999). Unfortunately, adolescents' participation in these after-school programs has lessened over the past two decades, largely because of reduced funding and because transportation and accessibility to these programs is often limited.
Colleges and universities are also a prime example of a community in which alcohol use is accepted and rampant. Despite college and university administration policies regarding underage drinking, the college environment continues to be a major time of alcohol use for youth. During college, underage youth can easily obtain alcohol from college mates older than age 21, and at times parents even supply college students with kegs of beer.
SUMMARY
Clearly, a great number of developmental and environmental factors contribute to underage drinking. All of these influences, from the most basic pubertal maturation to the most macrolevel societal policies, interact in complex ways. It must be recognized that behavior does not occur in a vacuum and cannot be understood adequately without considering the interplay between the individual and the many different types of environments in which he or she lives. For example, the confluence of cognitive and psychosocial maturation juxtaposed with societal expectations lead to shifts in youths' focus of affiliation gradually from parents to peers and from group relations to intimate relations with individuals outside of the family. These expanding social relationships broaden adolescents' sense of extra-familial reality and reinforce their increasing sense of individuality, need for autonomy, and desire to take on adult roles. Their newly acquired ability to think abstractly and to take a third person's perspective are important prerequisites for successful socialization that is tied to new responsibilities and freedom. However, such developments may also lead to risky choices, such as alcohol consumption.
Although we cannot change development, we can understand it, and we can use knowledge about adolescent and young adult development to inform the creation of more effective intervention programs. Knowledge about the development of future perspective taking, for example, is important to the development of alcohol-related prevention and intervention programs. Programs that focus on long-term outcomes will be less applicable and less effective for a young adolescent of about age 12 for whom there is an immature sense of the future and future consequences.
In addition, we can focus efforts on the environmental influences and the confluence between factors that are predictors of alcohol use. For example, enhancing parental monitoring and involvement, whereby parents are more aware of their child's whereabouts, is likely to result in youth who are less likely to use alcohol. Alcohol-related expectations are also related to alcohol use, with alcohol use positively related to perceived benefits and negatively related to perceived risks. As pointed out by Hawkins, Catalano, and Miller (1992: 87), prevention programs should include a “developmentally adjusted, multiple-component risk-reduction strategy that cuts across traditional health, education, and human service delivery systems.”
REFERENCES
- Aas H, Klepp KI. Adolescents' alcohol use related to perceived norms. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 1992;33(4):315–325. [PubMed: 1287824]
- Ajzen I. From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In: Kuhl J, Bechmann J, editors. Action-control: From cognition to behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1985. pp. 11–39.
- Ary DV, Tildesley E, Hops H, Andrews J. The influence of parent, sibling, and peer modeling and attitudes on adolescent use of alcohol. International Journal of the Addictions. 1993;28(9):853–880. [PubMed: 8359945]
- Baer JS, Barr HM, Bookstein FL, Sampson PD, Streissguth AP. Prenatal alcohol exposure and family history of alcoholism in the etiology of adolescent alcohol problems. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1998;59(5):533–543. [PubMed: 9718105]
- Baltes PB. Theoretical propositions of lifespan developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology. 1987;23:611–626.
- Baltes PB, Reese HW, Lipsitt LP. Lifespan developmental psychology. Annual Review of Psychology. 1980;31:65–110. [PubMed: 7362217]
- Bandura A. Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1986.
- Bandura A. Social cognitive theory and exercise of control over HIV infection. In: DiClemente RJ, Peterson JL, editors. Preventing AIDS: Theories and methods of behavioral interventions. New York: Plenum Press; 1994. pp. 24–29.
- Bandura A. Self-efficacy: The exercise of self-control. New York: W.H. Freeman; 1997.
- Barnes GM, Farrell MP, Banerjee S. Family influences on alcohol abuse and other problem behaviors among black and white Americans in a general population sample. In: Boyd GM, Howard J, Zucker RA, editors. Alcohol problems among adolescents: Current directions in prevention research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1995. pp. 13–31.
- Barnes GM, Reifman AS, Farrell MP, Dintcheff BA. The effects of parenting on the development of adolescent alcohol misuse: A six-wave latent growth model. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 2000;62(1):175–186.
- Baron J. Thinking and deciding. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1988.
- Beck KH, Lockhart SJ. A model of parental involvement in adolescent drinking and driving. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1992;21:35–51. [PubMed: 24263681]
- Beck KH, Treiman KA. The relationship of social context of drinking, perceived social norms, and parental influence to various drinking patterns of adolescents. Addictive Behaviors. 1996;21(5):633–644. [PubMed: 8876762]
- Bell RM, Ellickson PL, Harrison ER. Do drug prevention effects persist into high school? How Project ALERT did with ninth graders. Preventive Medicine. 1993;22:463–483. [PubMed: 8415497]
- Beyth-Marom R, Austin L, Fischhoff B, Palmgren C, Jacobs-Quadrel M. Perceived consequences of risky behaviors: Adults and adolescents. Developmental Psychology. 1993;29:549–563.
- Bogenschneider K, Wu MY, Raffaelli M, Tsay JC. Parent influences on adolescent peer orientation and substance use: The interface of parenting practices and values. Child Development. 1998;69:1672–1688. [PubMed: 9914646]
- Chen MJ, Grube JW, Madden PA. Alcohol expectancies and adolescent drinking: Differential prediction of frequency, quantity, and intoxication. Addictive Behaviors. 1994;19:521–529. [PubMed: 7832010]
- Christiansen BA, Goldman MS, Inn A. Development of alcohol-related expectancies in adolescents: Separating pharmacological from social-learning influences. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1982;50:336–344. [PubMed: 7096736]
- Christiansen BA, Roehling P, Smith G, Goldman M. Using alcohol expectancies to predict adolescent drinking behavior after one year. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1989;57:93–99. [PubMed: 2925979]
- Cialdini RB, Reno RR, Kallgren CA. A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;58:1015–1026.
- Cohen DA, Richardson J, LaBree L. Parenting behaviors and the onset of smoking and alcohol use: A longitudinal study. Pediatrics. 1994;94:368–375. [PubMed: 8065865]
- Colder CR, Chassin L. The psychosocial characteristics of alcohol users versus problem users: Data from a study of adolescents at risk. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:321–348. [PubMed: 16506537]
- Connell JP, Halpern-Felsher BL. How neighborhoods affect educational outcomes in middle childhood and adolescence: Conceptual issues and an empirical example. In: Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan G, Aber JL, editors. Neighborhood poverty, volume I: Context and consequences for children. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 1997. pp. 174–199.
- Connell JP, Halpern-Felsher BL, Clifford E, Crichlow W, Usinger P. Hanging in there: Behavioral, psychological, and contextual factors affecting whether African-American adolescents stay in high school. Journal of Adolescent Research. 1995;10:41–63.
- Connor JP, Young RM, Williams RJ, Ricciardelli LA. Drinking restraint versus alcohol expectancies: Which is the better indicator of alcohol problems? Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2000;61:352–359. [PubMed: 10757148]
- Curran PJ, Stice E, Chassin L. The relation between adolescent alcohol use and peer alcohol use: A longitudinal random coefficients model. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1997;65:130–140. [PubMed: 9103742]
- De Rosenroll DA. Early adolescent egocentrism: A review of six articles. Adolescence. 1987;22:791–802. [PubMed: 3324660]
- DiClemente RJ, Wingood GM, Crosby R, Sionean C, Cobb BK, Harrington K, Davies S, Hook EW III, Oh MK. Parental monitoring: Association with adolescents' risk behaviors. Pediatrics. 2001;107:1363–1368. [PubMed: 11389258]
- Dielman TE, Butchart AT, Shope JT, Miller M. Environmental correlates of adolescent substance use and misuse—Implications for prevention programs. International Journal of the Addictions. 1991;25(7A-8A):855–880. [PubMed: 2131323]
- Dielman TE, Shope JT, Butchart AT, Campanelli PC, Caspar RA. A covariance structure model test of antecedents of adolescent alcohol misuse and a prevention effort. Journal of Drug Education. 1989;19:337–361. [PubMed: 2621540]
- Dolcini MM, Cohn LD, Adler NE, Millstein SG, Irwin CE, Kegeles SM, Stone GC. Adolescent egocentrism and feelings of invulnerability: Are they related? Journal of Early Adolescence. 1989;9:409–418.
- Duncan TE, Duncan SC, Hops H. The effects of family cohesiveness and peer encouragement on the development of adolescent alcohol-use: A cohort-sequential approach to the analysis of longitudinal data. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1994;55:588–599. [PubMed: 7990469]
- Dunn ME, Goldman MS. Empirical modeling of an alcohol expectancy memory network in elementary school children as a function of grade. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1996;4:209–217.
- Eccles JS, Barber BL. Student council, volunteering, basketball, or marching band: What kind of extracurricular involvement matters? Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1999;6:281–294.
- Elkind D. Egocentrism in adolescence. Child Development. 1967;38:1025–1034. [PubMed: 5583052]
- Elkind D. Understanding the young adolescent. Adolescence. 1978;13:127–134.
- Ellickson PL, Bell RM, Harrison ER. Changing adolescent propensities to use drugs: Results from Project ALERT. Health Education Quarterly. 1993;20(2):227–242. [PubMed: 8491635]
- Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Lynskey MT. The prevalence and risk-factors associated with abusive or hazardous alcohol-consumption in 16-year-olds. Addiction. 1995;90:935–946. [PubMed: 7663315]
- Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Beliefs, attitudes, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley; 1975.
- Goldberg JH, Halpern-Felsher BL, Millstein SG. Beyond invulnerability: The importance of benefits in adolescents' decision to drink alcohol. Health Psychology. 2002;21:477–484. [PubMed: 12211515]
- Gray W, Hudson L. Formal operations and the imaginary audience. Developmental Psychology. 1984;20:619–627.
- Grube JW, Chen M, Madden P, Morgan M. Predicting adolescent drinking from alcohol expectancy values: A comparison of additive, interactive, and nonlinear models. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1995;25:839–857.
- Halebsky MA. Adolescent alcohol and substance abuse: Parent and peer effects. Adolescence. 1987;22:961–967. [PubMed: 3434409]
- Halpern-Felsher BL, Connell JP, Spencer MB, Aber JL, Duncan GP, Clifford E, Crichlow WE, Usinger PA, Cole SP, Allen L, Seidman E. Neighborhood and family factors predicting educational risk and attainment in African American and white children and adolescents. In: Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan G, Aber JL, editors. Neighborhood poverty, volume I: Context and consequences for children. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 1997. pp. 146–173.
- Halpern-Felsher BL, Millstein SG, Ellen J, Adler N, Tschann J, Biehl M. The role of behavioral experience in judging risks. Health Psychology. 2001;20:120–126. [PubMed: 11315729]
- Hampson SE, Severson HH, Burns WJ, Slovic P, Fisher KJ. Risk perception, personality factors and alcohol use among adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30:167–181.
- Hawkins JD, Catalano RF, Miller JY. Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other drug problems in adolescence and early adulthood: Implications for substance-abuse prevention. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112:64–105. [PubMed: 1529040]
- Hays RD, Ellickson PL. What is adolescent alcohol misuse in the United States according to the experts? Alcohol and Alcoholism. 1996;31:297–303. [PubMed: 8844036]
- Hill JP. Some perspectives on adolescence in American society. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare Office of Child Development; 1973.
- Jahnke HC, Blanchard-Fields F. A test of two models of adolescent egocentrism. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1993;22:313–326.
- Johnson V, Pandina RJ. A longitudinal examination of the relationships among stress, coping strategies, and problems associated with alcohol use. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1993;17:696–702. [PubMed: 8333603]
- Jones BT, Corbin W, Fromme K. A review of expectancy theory and alcohol consumption. Addiction. 2001;96:427–436. [PubMed: 11177520]
- Kallgren CA, Reno RR, Cialdini RB. A focus theory of normative conduct: When norms do and do not affect behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2000;26:1002–1012.
- Kanfer FH. Self-regulation: Research, issues and speculations. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1970.
- Keefe K. Perceptions of normative social pressure and attitudes toward alcohol-use: Changes during adolescence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1994;55:46–54. [PubMed: 8189725]
- Kuther TL. Rational decision perspectives on alcohol consumption by youth—Revising the theory of planned behavior. Addictive Behaviors. 2002;27:35–47. [PubMed: 11800223]
- Lamborn SD, Mounts NS, Steinberg L, Dornbusch SM. Patterns of competence and adjustment among adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Development. 1991;62:1049–1065. [PubMed: 1756655]
- Lapsley D, Milstead M, Quintana SM, Flannery D, Buss RR. Adolescent egocentrism and formal operations: Tests of a theoretical assumption. Developmental Psychology. 1986;22:800–807.
- Larson RW, Verma S. How children and adolescents spend time across the world: Work, play, and developmental opportunities. Psychological Bulletin. 1999;125:701–736. [PubMed: 10589300]
- Lee CY, Halpern-Felsher BL. Parenting of adolescents. In: Balter L, editor. Parenthood in America: An Encyclopedia. Vol. 1. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO; 2001. pp. 14–17.
- Lerner RM, Miller JR, Knott JH, et al. Integrating scholarship and outreach in human development research, policy, and service: A developmental contextual perspec-tive. In: Featherman DL, Lerner RM, Perlmutter M, editors. Life-span development and behavior. Vol. 12. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates; 1994. pp. 249–273.
- McGue M, Sharma A, Benson P. Parent and sibling influences on adolescent alcohol use and misuse: Evidence from a U.S. adoption cohort. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1996;57:8–18. [PubMed: 8747496]
- Miller PM, Smith GT, Goldman MS. Emergence of alcohol expectancies in childhood: A possible critical period. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1990;51:343–349. [PubMed: 2359308]
- Oei TP, Fergusson S, Lee NK. The differential role of alcohol expectancies and drinking refusal self-efficacy in problem and nonproblem drinkers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1998;59:704–711. [PubMed: 9811092]
- Olds RS, Thombs DL. The relationship of adolescent perceptions of peer norms and parent involvement to cigarette and alcohol use. Journal of School Health. 2001;71:223–228. [PubMed: 11512489]
- Pandina RJ, Johnson V. Familial drinking history as a predictor of alcohol and drug consumption among adolescent children. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1989;50:245–253. [PubMed: 2724972]
- Pandina RJ, Johnson V. Serious alcohol and drug problems among adolescents with a family history of alcoholism. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1990;51:278–282. [PubMed: 2342368]
- Parke RD, Ladd GW, editors. Family-peer relationships: Modes of linkage. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1992.
- Prendergast L. Substance use and abuse among college students: A review of the recent literature. Journal of American College Health. 1994;43:99–113. [PubMed: 7814772]
- Quadrel MJ, Fischhoff B, Davis W. Adolescent (in)vulnerability. American Psychologist. 1993;48:102–116. [PubMed: 8442566]
- Reifman A, Barnes GM, Dintcheff BA, Farrell MP, Uhteg L. Parental and peer influences on the onset of heavier drinking among adolescents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1998;59:311–317. [PubMed: 9598712]
- Rosenstock IM. Historical origins of the health belief model. In: Becker MH, editor. The health belief model and personal health behavior. Thorofare, NJ: Charles B. Sclack; 1974. pp. 1–8.
- Sieving RE, Perry CL, Williams CL. Do friendships change behaviors, or do behaviors change friendships? Examining paths of influence in young adolescents' alcohol use. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2000;26:27–35. [PubMed: 10638715]
- Silverberg SB, Gondoli DM. Autonomy in adolescence: A contextualized perspective. In: Adams R, Montemayor R, Gullotta TP, editors. Psychosocial development during adolescence: Progress in developmental contextualism. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1996. pp. 12–61.
- Smith G, Goldman M, Greenbaum P, Christiansen B. Expectancy for social facilitation from drinking: The divergent paths of high-expectancy and low-expectancy adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1995;104:32–40. [PubMed: 7897051]
- Steinberg L, Cauffman E. Maturity of judgment in adolescence: Psychosocial factors in adolescent decision making. Law and Human Behavior. 1996;20:249–272.
- Steinberg LD, Fletcher A, Darling N. Parental monitoring and peer influences on adolescent substance use. Pediatrics. 1994;93:1060–1064. [PubMed: 8197008]
- Stice E, Barrera M, Chassin L. Prospective differential prediction of adolescent alcohol use and problem use: Examining the mechanisms of effect. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1998;107:616–628. [PubMed: 9830249]
- Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. National household survey on drug abuse. Rockville, MD: Author; 2001.
- Triandis HC. Interpersonal behavior. Monterey, CA: Brooks Cole; 1977.
- Urberg K, Robbins R. Perceived vulnerability in adolescents to the health consequences of cigarette smoking. Preventive Medicine. 1984;13:367–376. [PubMed: 6504865]
- Vakalahi HF. Adolescent substance use and family-based risk and protective factors: A literature review. Journal of Drug Education. 2001;31:29–46. [PubMed: 11338964]
- Webb JA, Baer PR. Influence of family disharmony and parental alcohol-use on adolescent social skills, self-efficacy, and alcohol-use. Addictive Behaviors. 1995;20:127–135. [PubMed: 7785478]
- Webster RA, Hunter M, Keats JA. Personality and sociodemographic influences on adolescents' substance use: A path-analysis. International Journal of the Addictions. 1994;29:941–956. [PubMed: 8050836]
- Weinberg NZ, Dielman TE, Mandell W, Shope JT. Parental drinking and gender factors in the prediction of early adolescent alcohol-use. International Journal of the Addictions. 1994;29:89–104. [PubMed: 8144271]
- Weinstein M, Fineberg H. Clinical decision analysis. Philadelphia: Saunders; 1980.
- Weinstein ND. Reducing unrealistic optimism about illness. Health Psychology. 1983;2:11–20.
- Weisenberg M, Kegeles SS, Lund AK. Children's health beliefs and acceptance of a dental preventive activity. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1980;21:59–74. [PubMed: 7365231]
- Wood MD, Nagoshi CT, Dennis DA. Alcohol norms and expectations as predictors of alcohol use and problems in a college student sample. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 1992;18:461–476. [PubMed: 1449125]
Footnotes
- 1
We do not discuss factors that influence excessive or atypical alcohol use (e.g., mental or addictive disorders) because the factors generally responsible for these drinking patterns are not necessarily the same as those involved in normative alcohol use.
- PubMedLinks to PubMed
- Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overv...Developmental and Environmental Influences on Underage Drinking: A General Overview - Reducing Underage Drinking
Your browsing activity is empty.
Activity recording is turned off.
See more...